Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25-42.
[4 referring publications by Place]
Posner, M. I., & Warren, R. E. (1972). Traces, concepts and conscious construction. In A. W. Melton, & E. Martin (Eds.) Coding Processes in Human Memory. Winston.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Povinelli, D. J., & Davis, D. R. (1994). Differences between chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens) in the resting state of the finger: implications for pointing. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 134-139
[1 referring publications by Place]
Powell, J. P. (1969). The brain and consciousness: a reply to Professor Burt. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 22, 27-28.
[Abstract]This article presents a criticism of C. Burt's article. Essentially, Burt's "mental field" theory is questioned in terms of its plausibility and scientific status. It is argued that the "astonishing complexity of the brain may well baffle us, but our lack of understanding can scarcely be ameliorated by mystery-mongering.
[Citing Place (1956)] [Is reply to] [1 referring publications by Place] [Is replied by]
Price, H. H. (1932). Perception Methuen.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Price, H. H. (1953). Thinking and experience Hutchinson.
[8 referring publications by Place]
Pringle-Patterson, A. S. (Ed.) (1924). Locke's Essay Concerning Human Understanding Clarendon Press.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Prior, A. N. (1957). Time and modality Clarendon Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Prior, A. N. (1968). Intentionality and Intensionality II. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society Supplementary Volumes, LXII, 91-106.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Proust M. (1923). À la recherche du temps perdu (8 volumes). Éditions de la nouvelle Revue Française
[1 referring publications by Place]
Pryor, K. (1984). Don't shoot the dog! Bantam Books.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Putnam, H. (1960). Minds and machines. In S. Hook (Ed.), Dimensions of mind. Collier Books.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Putnam, H. (1962). Dreaming and 'depth grammar.' In R.J. Butler (ed.), Analytical Philosophy (First Series, pp. 211-235), Blackwell.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Putnam, H. (1964). Robots: Machines or artificially created life? The Journal of Philosophy, 61, 668-691.
[1 referring publications by Place] [1 reprinting collections]
Putnam, H. (1967). Psychological Predicates. In W. H. Capitan and D. D. Merill, (Eds), Art, Mind and Religion (pp. 37-48). University of Pittsburgh Press.
[3 referring publications by Place] [Is replied by] [1 reprinting collections]
Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language and reality, Philosophical papers, Vol. 2. Cambridge University Press.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of 'meaning'. In K. Gunderson (Ed.) Language, Mind and Knowledge, Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science (VII). University of Minnesota Press.
[7 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1948). On what there is, Review of Metaphysics. Reprinted in From a Logical Point of View, Harvard University Press, 1953, Chapter I.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1951). Two dogmas of empiricism. Philosophical Review, LX. Reprinted in W. v. O Quine (1953), From a logical point of view. Harvard University Press.
[14 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1953). On mental entities. Proceedings of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 80(3), 198-203.
[1 referring publications by Place] [1 reprinting collections]
Quine, W. v. O. (1953). From a logical point of view. Harvard University Press.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1953). Reference and modality. In From a Logical Point of View (chapter VIII, pp. 139-159). Harvard University Press.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1960). Word and Object. M.I.T. Press.
[6 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. (1969). Epistemology naturalized. In Ontological Relativity and Other Essays. Columbia University Press.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Quine, W. v. O. & Ullian, I. S. (1970). The Web of Belief. Random House.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Quinlan (1991). Connectionism and Psychology. Chicago University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Quinton, A. (1973). The Nature of Things Routledge & Kegan Paul.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rachlin, H. (1985). Pain and behavior. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 43-53.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rafal, R., & Robertson, L. (1995). The neurology of visual attention. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (Ch. 40, pp. 625-648). MIT Press, .
[3 referring publications by Place]
Raichle, M. E., Fiez, J. A., Videen, T. O., MacLeod, A.-M. K., Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T., & Petersen, S. E. (1994). Practice-related changes in human functional anatomy during non-motor learning. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 8-26.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Ravetz, J. R. (1971). Scientific knowledge and its social problems Clarendon Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Reeke, G. N., & Edelman, G. M. (1988). Real brains and artificial intelligence. Daedalus, 117, 143-173.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rees, W. L. (1976). Stress, Distress and Disease. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 128()1, 3-18.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rees, W. R. (1957-8). Continuous States. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 58, 223-244
[1 referring publications by Place]
Reese, H. W. (1992). Problem solving by algorithms and heuristics. In S. C. Hayes, & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding Verbal Relations (Chapter 10, pp. 153-179). Context Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In L. R. Goulet, & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-293850-4.50011-X
[Abstract]Models, which originate in metaphor, exist on several levels ranging from all inclusive metaphysical models to narrowly circumscribed models of specific features of theories. Models at the more general levels form the determining logical context for models at lower levels. This categorical determinism stretches from metaphysical levels through scientific theories, to the manner in which we analyze, interpret, and make inferences from empirical evidence. Two radically different models which have had a pervasive effect upon the nature of psychology generally and developmental psychology specifically are the organismic and mechanistic world views. The history and nature of those models are discussed and the manner in which they become transformed into corrolary issues which form the metatheoretical basis for theory construction is analyzed. Theories built upon different world views are logically independent and cannot be assimilated to each other. They reflect different ways of looking at the world and, as such, are incompatible in their implications.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rescorla, R. A. (1991). Associative relations in instrumental learning: The eighteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 1-23.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. In A. H. Black, & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical Conditioning, Vol. 2: Current Research and Theory. Prentice-Hall.
[7 referring publications by Place]
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188-194.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Robinson, G. M. (1977). Procedures for the acquisition of syntax. In W. K. Honig, & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of Operant Behavior. Prentice-Hall.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rockwell, W. T. (1994). On what the mind is identical with. Philosophical Psychology, 7(3), 307–323. doi:10.1080/09515089408573126
[Abstract]The unity of mind and body need not imply accepting the unity of mind and brain, because the mind‐brain identity is something that science has presupposed, not discovered. I cite evidence from modern neuroscience that cognitive activities are distributed throughout the human nervous system, which challenges the ‘scientific’ assumption (believed by Descartes, among others) that the brain is the seat of the soul, and the rest of the nerves are mere message cables to the brain. Dennett comes close to accepting this point when he criticizes ‘Cartesian materialism’, and yet he still claims that Vie head is headquarters’. Accepting that the mind is the entire nervous system solves some philosophical problems, for Dennett and others. There is also some evidence that indicates that some cognitive activities may be hormonal rather than neural, which raises some challenging problems for the once obvious distinction between causing a mental state and embodying that state.
[Citing Place (1956)]
Romanes, G. J. (1882). Animal Intelligence. Kegan Paul, Trench & Co.
[4 referring publications by Place]
Romanes, G. J. (1888). Mental evolution in man: Origin of human faculty Kegan Paul.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Rorschach, H. (1932/1942). Psychodiagnostik Hans Huber. English translation as Psychodiagnostics by P. Lemkau & B. Kronenberg, ed. W. Morganthaler. Grune & Stratton.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Rorty, R. (1965). Mind-brain identity, privacy, and categories. The Review of Metaphysics, xix, 24-54.
[5 referring publications by Place] [1 reprinting collections]
Rosenblatt, F. (1959). Two theorems of statistical separability in the perceptron. In Mechanisation of Thought Processes: Proceedings of a Symposium held at the National Physical Laboratory, November 1958. Vol. 1, (pp. 421-456). HM Stationery Office.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Rosenblueth, A., Wiener, N. & Bigelow, J. (1943). Behavior, Purpose and Teleology. Philosophy of Science, 10, 18-24.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Rossetti, Y. (1997). Implicit perception in action: short-lived motor representations in space. In P.G. Grossenbacher (Ed.), Advances in Consciousness Research. John Benjamins.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., & Boisson, D. (1995). Implicit processing of somesthetic information: a dissociation between Where and How? Neuroreport, 6(3), 506-510.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Rubin, E. (1915). Synsoplevede Figurer. Gyldendalska
[2 referring publications by Place]
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L, & the PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Volumes 1 and 2). MIT Press.
[6 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1900). A Critical Exposition of the Philosophy of Leibniz Allen & Unwin.
[4 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1903). The Principles of Mathematics. Allen and Unwin.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1914). The relation of sense-data to physics. Scientia, 16, 1-27. No.4 reprinted in Mysticism and Logic, Longmans and Green, 1917, Chap.VIII.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1918/1919). The philosophy of logical atomism. The Monist, xxviii, 495-527; xxix, 32‑63; xxix, 190-222; xxix, 345-380. Reprinted in B. Russell (1956), Logic and Knowledge, Essays 1901-1950 (Edited by R. C. Marshall). Allen and Unwin.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1921). The Analysis of Mind Macmillan.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. (1940). Inquiry into meaning and truth. Allen and Unwin.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. A. W. (1905). On Denoting. Mind, 4, 479-493
[1 referring publications by Place]
Russell, B. A. W. (1919). Introduction to mathematical philosophy Allen and Unwin.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Ryle, G. (1938). Categories. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 38, 189-206.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Ryle, G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. Hutchinson.
[83 referring publications by Place]
Ryle, G. (1954). Dilemmas. Cambridge University Press.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Ryle, G. (1958). A puzzling element in the notion of thinking. Proceedings of the British Academy, XLIV, 129-144.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Sacks, H. (1972). On the analysability of stories by children. In J. J. Gumpertz, & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics: The ethnography of communication (pp. 329-345).
[Abstract]Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sacks, H. (1973). The preference for agreement in natural conversation. Paper presented to the Summer Institute of Linguistics, Ann Arbor, 1973.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sartre, J. P. (1962). Sketch for a Theory of Emotions Methuen.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1986). Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol Columbia University Press
[3 referring publications by Place]
Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Cambridge University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schegloff, E. A. (1968). Sequencing in conversational openings. American Anthropologist, 70, 1075-1095.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Schegloff, E. A. (1982). Discourse as an interactional achievement: some uses of "Uh huh" and other things that come between sentences. In D.Tannen (Ed.), Georgetown University Roundtable on Languages and Linguistics 1981. Georgetown University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up Closings. Semiotica, 8(4), 289-327. doi:10.1515/semi.1973.8.4.289
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schilpp, P. A. (Ed.) (1942). The philosophy of G. E. Moore. North-western University and Cambridge University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schlick, M. (1932). Positivismus und Realismus Erkenntnis, 3, 1-31
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schlick, M. (1935). Facts and propositions. Analysis, 2, 5.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schusterman, R. J., & Gisiner, R. C. (1988). Artificial language comprehension in dolphins and sea lions: The essential cognitive skills. The Psychological Record, 38, 311-348.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Schusterman, R. J., & Gisiner, R. C. (1989). Please parse the sentence: Animal cognition in the procrustean bed of linguistics. The Psychological Record, 39, 3-18.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (1993). A California sea lion (Zalophus
californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 43, 823-839.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Schusterman, R. J., & Krieger, K. (1984). California sea lions are capable of semantic comprehension. The Psychological Record, 34, 3-23.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech Acts. Cambridge University Press.
[3 referring publications by Place]
Searle, J. R. (1979). Intentionality and the use of language. In A. Margalit (Ed.), Meaning and Use. Reidel.
[2 referring publications by Place]
Searle, J. R. (1979). What is an Intentional State? Mind, LXXXVIII, 74-92.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains and programs. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417-424.
[3 referring publications by Place] [1 reprinting collections]
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: an essay on the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press.
[10 referring publications by Place]
Searle, J. R. (1984). Minds, Brains and Science: The 1984 Reith Lectures. British Broadcasting Corporation.
[4 referring publications by Place] [Is replied by]
Searle, J. R. (1992). The Rediscovery of Mind. M.I.T. Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness. Freeman.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sellars, W. (1963). Science, Perception and Reality Routledge & Kegan Paul.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sellars, W., & Chisholm, R. M. (1958). Appendix: Intentionality and the mental. In Concepts, theories, and the mind-body problem. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (Volume 2, pp. 507-539). University of Minnesota Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Shakespeare, W, (1606). King Lear.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sheinberg, D. L. & Logothetis, N. K. (1997). The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 94, 3408-3413.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sherrard, C. (1987, December). Rhetorical weapons: Chomsky's attack on Skinner [Conference presentation]. Symposium on Discourse Analysis, London Conference of the British Psychological Society.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sherringtion, C. S. (1940). Man on his Nature, Gifford Lectures 1937-8. Cambridge University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Shoemaker, S. (1963). Self Knowledge and Self Identity Cornell University Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of Scientific Research. Basic Books.
[5 referring publications by Place]
Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and audio-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5-13.
[7 referring publications by Place]
Sidman, M. (1977). Teaching some basic prerequisites for reading. In P. Mittler (ed.), Research to Practice in Mental Retardation (II). University Park Press.
[1 referring publications by Place]
Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioural units. Erlbaum.
[4 referring publications by Place]
Sidman, M. (1990). Equivalence relations: Where do they come from? In D. E. Blackman, & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 92-114). Erlbaum.
[4 referring publications by Place]